Picking up a ball's spin might help a player judge how the ball will break,
but Cathy Craig, a psychologist at Queen's University in Belfast, Northern
Ireland, has bad news for players: Their visual systems can't process this
information.
Craig enlisted 11 players and nine goalkeepers from elite European
professional teams to determine how well players can interpret how spin affects
the ball's flight. Each player donned a virtual reality helmet that gave the
perspective of standing in the center of the goal. They could look around the
stadium, but there was no visual clutter, just a ball sitting 33 yards straight
ahead, about the location of the average free kick.
When the ball was virtually kicked toward the player with no spin, but
still having gravity affecting its flight, the players consistently judged
accurately whether it would enter the goal. But when the ball spun clockwise or
counterclockwise, even the expert goal keepers were lost.
"Gravity is constant and we've known how to deal with it since we were
born," Craig told LiveScience. "But we're not used to dealing with spin, and our
nervous system is better at dealing with invariance than things that vary all
the time."
In the future, Craig suggests goalkeepers might prepare for an opponent
by using a similar virtual device to accustom themselves with the type of spin
the opposing free kick specialist puts on the ball.
It might also be good testing ground for manufacturers developing new
balls. Sometimes engineers design a ball that flies well in a wind tunnel, but
players hate it because the pattern painted on it makes it appear to wobble in
flight. Trying out new paint jobs in a virtual setting could help companies
develop a ball that both physicists and players love.
Offside fools
Soccer referees draw plenty of criticism from fans, but incorrectly calling
"offside" tends to be the source of extreme ire.
The offside rule states that no offensive player is allowed to run past a
defender and then touch the ball unless the ball is already in front of them.
What makes it tricky is a player can run past a defender to receive a pass as
long as the player has not passed the defender when the ball was kicked.
A wrong call one way can kill a team's legitimate chance of scoring, and a
bad call the other way can allow a cheap goal.
Francisco Belda Maruenda, a physician and researcher in Spain, would tell you
not to be so hard of the referees, since they're being asked to perform a
physiologically impossible task.
The ability of the eye to change focus on a far object to one located less
than 6 yards away is called eye accommodation. For most people, it takes around
600 milliseconds. Since the players and ball are spread all over the field, a
referee almost always performs eye accommodation when making an offside call.
But according to Maruenda, the average running player can move roughly 5 feet
in the time it takes for the ref's eyes to refocus, so 600 milliseconds is just
too slow.
To make an accurate call, "it is necessary to stop time and to locate all the
players who take part in that game in zero milliseconds," Maruenda told
LiveScience. To watch the player making the pass, the player receiving the pass,
the defender, and the ball at the same time is just too much for our visual
systems to handle, especially from the close-up view of the referee.
Some soccer officials have suggested giving a referee a bird's-eye view of
the game and freeze-frame television technology to make better offside calls.
Until then, it's worth noting that fans who bash refs for bad calls suffer the
same blindness to the truth.